Appendix 2: extra task on note-taking

Make photocopies of the text below. Use this extra task at the most suitable stage in Unit 2 where your students will benefit most.

TASK

Read Text A on the following pages and consider how easy it would be to take notes from it. Try taking notes on it yourself, allowing a maximum time limit of five minutes. Then look at the information in Text B and try taking notes on it in the same amount of time.

Text A

The notion that intelligence can be measured dates back to the end of the nineteenth century. Since at that time there was very little knowledge about how the brain works, the crudity of the first attempts is perhaps not surprising. The early experimental psychologists sought to measure brain capacity by measuring the size of the head.

It was disillusion with the above method that led the Frenchman Alfred Binet to devise the first intelligence tests. In 1904, he was asked by the French government to invent techniques that could identify those children in state schools who needed special help. This had a dual purpose. Firstly, it was intended to avoid the holding back of progress in a class because some children in it were incapable of learning at a given standard. Secondly, it was designed to save public money.

The original Binet-Simon IQ scale (1905) was revised twice before Binet's untimely death in 1911 and attracted the interest of psychologists throughout the world. In fact, by 1911 the test was being used in 71 out of the 84 cities in the United States which administered tests to spot retarded children. The most famous revision of the Binet-Simon Scale in America was developed under the direction of L. M. Terman at Stanford University and became known as the Stanford-Binet Scale (Terman, 1916). This was later revised by Terman and Merrill in 1937 and has subsequently been updated again (Terman and Merrill, 1937, 1960, 1973).

Besides the Stanford-Binet tests and their revisions, some of the most widely used of modern IQ tests were devised in the United States by David Wechsler. The first version of the Wechsler-Bellevue Intelligence Scale was published in 1939. One of the principal aims in its preparation was to provide an intelligence test suitable for adults rather than schoolchildren, which had been the case of previous work.

Though modern tests based on the approach originally devised by Binet and later modified by others continue to be used, there has for some years been considerable controversy over their validity.

Early proponents of testing often made exaggerated claims about the validity of their results. Some suggested that Intelligence was an absolute value like height or weight. Others claimed that it was uniform and unchanging throughout the world.

Criticisms of IQ testings were, in the first instance, undoubtedly a reaction to such extreme positions. However, they have also emerged due to the disturbing potential of test results as a tool of discrimination against groups who have scored less well on average than other groups. This fact has often led to justifiable accusations of cultural bias in test content.

Some recent views of intelligence have suggested that traditional testing may only account for a limited number of abllities. Modern research has revealed the immense complexity of the brain but we know little more about how it works than we did when measuring head size was taken seriously. It may be that, as some clinical psychologists have pointed out, intelligence cannot be viewed separately from personality as a whole.

Dissatisfaction with present methods will no doubt pave the way for new approaches. Wechsler's position on testing IQ reflects the views of modest modern-day proponents. He denied that any absolute value could be given to intelligence. Rather, he hoped that tests could give some indication of an individual's ability to understand and deal with the world. The particular skills, such as quantitative or verbal ability, measured by the component part of his test did not interest him.

Contemporary defence of the use of IQ tests is based on modest claims. Firstly, selection is an inescapable feature of most societies. The need for criteria to help identify less capable students or choose the right job applicant cannot be avoided.

Secondly, some traditional eritería of selection may not be reliable, for example, Judging by personal appeárance or membership of the right 'old school tie' network.

Thirdly, since they measure a mixture of cognitive skills rather than specific, acquired knowledge, IQ tests may have a higher degree of objectivity. This does not mean that they can be relied on as a sole indicator of ability but that they may have some value in supplemenling other criteria.

Text B

Modern IQ tests are largely based on the approach originally devised by Alfred Binet in France and later modified by others or, more recently, the scale pioneered by David Wechsler in the United States. Controversy regarding the validity of testing intelligence has tended to grow in recent years even though the approach is much more sophisticated than the first attempts at the end of the 19th century.

Before Binet was approached by the French government to invent techniques that could identify children requiring special help in state schools, attempts at measuring brain capacity focused on measuring the size of the head. Our knowledge of how the brain works is not much greater today than it was then, though we are more aware of its immense complexity. It may be that, even today, our perception of intelligence is very limited and that testing only accounts for a limitted number of abilities. Some clinical psychologists have maintained that intelligence cannot be viewed separately from personality as a whole.

Dissatisfaction with present methods will no doubt pave the way for new approaches just as it did in Binet's case when he became disillusioned with measuring heads. The appearance of the Binet-Simon IQ scale in 1905 was, in addition, propelled by the desire of the French government to save money and avoid holding back the progress of children in a class because some of them were not capable of learning at a given standard.

Binet's test and its subsequent revisions in France and the United States focused on measuring the intelligence of school-children. One of the principal aims of the Wedisler-Bellevue Intelligence Scale, published in 1939, was to provide a test suitable for adults.

Though Wechsler extended the application of tests, he was cautious in his claims. In his view, intelligence could never be measured to give an absolute value in the way that, for example, height and weight can. Moreover, he was not interested in particular skills measured by the component parts of his tests, such as quantitive or verbal ability. He hoped that tests could give some indication of an individual's ability to understand and deal with the world. His modest views reflect the position of more moderate advocates of testing today.

Prior to the Wechsler-Bellevue Scale, there had been several revisions of the Binet-Simon Scale following its founder's untimely death in 1911. By that time, his work had attracted the attention of psychologists throughout the world and was being used in 71 out of the 84 cities in the United States which administered tests to spot retarded children. The most well-known revision of the Binet-Simon Scale in America was-developed under the direction of L. M. Terman at Stanford University and became known as the Stanford-Binet Scale (Terman, 1916). Its most recent revision was in 1973, having previously been updated on two occasions (Terman and Merrill, 1937, 1960, 1973).

Some early proponents of testing made exaggerated claims about the validity of their results. It was sometimes ciaimed that intelligence was an absolute value which was uniform and unchanging throughout the world.

Criticisms of IQ testing were, in the first instance, undoubtedly a reaction to such extreme positions. However, they have also emerged due to the disturbing potential of test results as a tool of discrimination against groups who have scored less well on average than other groups. This fact has frequently led to justifiable accusations of cultural bias in text content.

Modern defence of the use of tests is based on modest claims. It can be argued that selection is an inescapable feature of most societies. The need for criteria to help identify less capable students or choose the right job applicant cannot be avoided. Some traditional criteria of selection, such as judging by appearance or membership of the right 'old school tie' network may not be as reliable as IQ tests. Since they measure a mixture of cognitive skills rather than specific, acquired knowledge, these tests may have a higher degree of objectivity. This does not mean that they can be relied on as a sole indicator of ability but that they may have some value in supplementing other criteria.

If you tried taking notes on the above two texts, you will have found that it was easier to do so from Text A. Although the content of the two texts is the same, in Text B, it is presented in a non-linear way making it difficult to extricate important

information.

A first 'skeleton' of notes on the above texts, identifying functions and some key information might look something like this:

History of measurement of intelligence
1. Late 19th century
2. Binet-Simon
(Backg'd) 3. Stanford-Binet
  4. Wechsler-Bellevue Scale (1939) (aimed at adults, unlike previous tests)
Arguments for and against IQ testing
For Against
Extreme Modest
 

Понравилась статья? Добавь ее в закладку (CTRL+D) и не забудь поделиться с друзьями:  



double arrow
Сейчас читают про: